Dragonflies on the hunt display complex choreography
The dragonfly is a swift and efficient hunter. Once it spots its prey, it takes about half a second to swoop beneath an unsuspecting insect and snatch it from the air. Scientists at the Howard Hughes Medical Institute's Janelia Research Campus have used motion-capture techniques to track the details of that chase, and found that a dragonfly's movement is guided by internal models of its own body and the anticipated movement of its prey. Similar internal models are used to guide behavior in humans. "This highlights the role that internal models play in letting these creatures construct such a complex behavior," says Janelia group leader Anthony Leonardo, who led the study. "It starts to reshape our view of the neural underpinnings of this behavior." Leonardo, postdoctoral fellows Matteo Mischiati and Huai-Ti Lin, and their colleagues published the findings in the December 11, 2014, issue of the journal Nature.
"Until now, this type of complex control, which incorporates both prediction and reaction, had been demonstrated only in vertebrates," writes Harvard University biologist Stacey A. Combes in an accompanying News & Views perspective in Nature. "However, Mischiati et al. show that dragonflies on the hunt perform internal calculations every bit as complex as those of a ballet dancer."
Neuroscientists have learned a lot about how the nervous system triggers actions in response to sensory information by studying simple reflexive behaviors, such as how an animal escapes a predator. Leonardo has been studying prey capture in dragonflies because he wants to know whether the same stimulus-response loops that researchers have uncovered in those systems also underlie more complex behaviors.
In humans, the simple act of reaching for an object demands sophisticated information processing, Leonardo says. Just to pick up a cup of coffee, the brain calls on a number of internal models. "You have an internal model of how your arm works, how the joints are articulated, of the cup and its mass. If the cup is filled with coffee, you incorporate that," he explains. "Articulating a body and moving it through space is a very complicated problem." Scientists had so far thought of prey capture by insects as a straightforward system in which a predator's movement is guided solely by the position its prey. "The idea was the dragonfly roughly knows where the prey is relative to him, and he tries to hold this angle constant as he moves toward the interception point. This is the way guided missiles work and how people catch footballs," Leonardo says. But there was reason to believe prey capture was more complicated.
"You don't need a spectacularly complicated model to guess where the prey will be a short time in the future," he says. "But how do you maneuver your body to reach the point of contact?"
In search of a more complete picture, Leonardo and his team spent several years devising a system that allows them to track a dragonfly's body movements as it intercepts its prey. Their strategy is based on the same motion-capture technology used to translate actors' movements into computer animation: reflective markers are placed on different body parts -- in this case, the head, body, and wings -- and a high-speed video camera records flashes of light reflected from each marker as the insect moves. Using the position of each flash of light, the scientists can reconstruct an outline of the dragonfly as it flies (the accuracy of the outline depends on the number of markers attached to the dragonfly).
Leonardo and his colleagues recorded dragonflies' movements as they chased after either a fruit fly or an artificial prey -- a bead maneuvered by a pulley system -- whose movements the scientists could precisely control. They focused on following the orientation of the dragonfly's head and body. "That tells us what the dragonfly sees and how its body moves," he explains.
When they analyzed their videos, it was clear that the dragonflies were not simply responding to the movements of the prey. Instead, they made structured turns that adjusted the orientation of their bodies -- even when their prey's trajectory did not change. "Those turns were driven by the dragonfly's internal representation of its body and the knowledge that it has to rotate its body and line it up to the prey's flight path in a particular way," Leonardo says.
The dragonflies always aligned themselves so that they would intercept their prey from below, reducing the risk of detection. "At the end of the chase, the fly makes a basket out its legs and the prey drops into it," Leonardo says.
Those shifts in orientation create a challenge for the predator. "The dragonfly is making a lot of turns to line itself up. Those turns create a lot of apparent prey motion. If the whole world is going to spin, how can it possibly see its prey?" Leonardo asks. Surprisingly, the scientists found that each dragonfly moved its head to keep the image of its prey centered on the eye, despite the rotation of its own body. The head movements happened too fast to be a reaction to visual disturbances created by the rotation of the dragonfly's body, Leonardo says. Instead, the head movements must be planned based on the insect's predictions about how to stabilize the image of its prey.
Leonardo says the movements his team observed are so fine-tuned that they keep the image of the prey fixed in the crosshairs of the dragonfly's eyes -- their area of greatest acuity -- during the duration of the chase. That allows the dragonfly to receive two channels of information about its prey, Leonardo says. The angle between the head and the body tracks the predicted movement of the prey, while the visual system detects any unexpected movement when the prey strays from its position in the crosshairs. "It gives the dragonfly a very elegant combination of predicted model-driven control and the original reactive control," he says.
Video: Dragonfly prey capture http://bcove.me/jln0dlu8
Source: Howard Hughes Medical Institute (HHMI)
Other sources
- Dragonflies on the hunt employ sophisticated maneuveringfrom UPIThu, 11 Dec 2014, 5:01:01 UTC
- Dragonflies on the hunt display complex choreographyfrom Science DailyWed, 10 Dec 2014, 21:31:18 UTC
- Dragonflies on the Hunt Display Complex Choreographyfrom Newswise - ScinewsWed, 10 Dec 2014, 19:00:54 UTC
- Dragonflies on the Hunt Are Smarter Than You Thinkfrom MSNBC: ScienceWed, 10 Dec 2014, 19:00:12 UTC
- Dragonflies on the hunt display complex choreographyfrom PhysorgWed, 10 Dec 2014, 18:00:26 UTC